“Serifs improve fast, easy readability for long text” – myth or truth?

It’s an old question, but an important one. Let’s see if we can get a good, nuanced, ideally evidence-based answer.

So, old-school wisdom (certainly, how I was taught back in the day) says that serif text improves the readability of long passages of text. The eye passes over the text more easily, there is less “fatigue” on the eye, and reading speed is improved. As I was taught, this is the reason why book typesetters almost always use moderately florid serifs like Garamond. Sans-serifs, according to traditional wisdom, are better for legibility – the letters are simpler, less room for error – and so are better suited for short text, like road signs.

Within the last decade or so – certainly since reading on screens became commonplace – I’ve seen an increasingly common viewpoint that this is an outdated myth – that actually, serifs are faster for reading long text for no reason other than that we are historically accustomed to reading long passages of serif text, and that long passages of well typeset, well chosen sans can be just as good for readability and fast reading, as people become accustomed to it.

There’s also a third viewpoint I’m aware of, which says that the second viewpoint is a myth that comes from the fact that serif fonts tend not to reduce well on pixel screens, making sans type the better (least worst) choice for long passages of on-screen text or poorly-printed reproductions, but serifs still the best for long passages of printed type. Hence the popularity of websites with sans body text and serif headers, and of printed materials with serif body text and sans headers. Essentially, it characterises the arguments for the second viewpoint as merely pointing out that good (well produced) sans type is better than bad serif type, and maintains that, for extended reading, all other things being equal, good serif type is better than good sans type.

And finally, there’s a fourth viewpoint that it doesn’t matter anyway – that there are no differences between serifs in general and sans in general that are not merely artefacts of the differences between the example fonts and typesetting used in any particular test or comparison. There’s certainly a case to be made for this in the context of legibility, but I’ve not seen anything convincing on this for readability.

All other things being equal, do serifs on a typeface genuinely make lengthy body text easier (faster and less effortful) to read?

Readability (as defined by the speed of error-free reading) is something objective that can be, and has been, measured. This is a factual question. Can we give it a solid, rounded, nuanced factual answer?


A couple of asides relating to common arguments I’ve seen:

  • There’s an old-fashioned justification for serifs based on the idea that the eye follows a line of text, and the serifs, by hinting at a cohesive horizontal, help the eye along its way. The first part of this is simply not true – the eye moves in numerous extremely fast jumps (‘saccades’) we are not consciously aware of, and the motion between jumps is too fast for any information to be taken in. It’s ‘Jump-focus-jump-focus-jump-focus’, leading to one awareness of the area covered by the jumps. However, this doesn’t completely nullify the gist of the argument. It’s perfectly possible, for example, that serifs could help create implied cohesive horizontals in the blurred areas of vision outside the fovea which could help the reading process by guiding saccades and/or making word boundaries more distinct.

  • A point is sometimes made about serifs being a cultural artefact. That’s undoubtedly true – but it’s interesting to note that many scripts have optional equivalents to serifs which, like Roman serifs, mark significant protrusions and corners, and which in some cases have a history that can’t be put down to simply following popular roman typefaces. For example, they’re historically important in Chinese (and therefore also Japanese) writing, and interestingly, give a sense of direction that is both horizontal and vertical (makes sense as historically these have been written in a variety of directions). So, it’s not grounds for dismissing serifs as just an artefact of one cultural heritage.

enter image description here


Finally, a related topic has been touched on over at the UX site, and second answer has many interesting references, but that question has no clear focus between readability and legibility, and so doesn’t give a definitive answer on the readability front.

Answer

Summed up into a couple of points, here are my thoughts on the subject.

  1. “Readability” is also about what we are most familiar with. English speakers tend to be familiar with both serif and sans-serif typefaces, enough to be able to read both extremely fluently. You could say that most of our most lengthy reading (eg, novels, newspapers) uses traditional/transitional serif faces, so the argument could be made that we ought to be slightly more at ease with these particular serifs. But in actuality, we’re very good at reading common sans-serif fonts too, to the point where we’re good enough at both for any individual differences to be unsubstantial.

    As for people who use other alphabets than our Latin alphabet, this will be different. There are some alphabets/scripts which do not have a serif/sans-serif equivalent or if they do, one is rarely used, in which case the more common one will be the more readable form.

  2. The technical specifics of a low resolution computer screen (ie, not a retina display/high resolution smartphone) have always limited what we can do with type in that medium, and for various reasons sans-serif became the norm there for a long time. The reason for sans-serif being regarded as more “readable” on a low-resolution computer screen is more to do with technical aspects; serif fonts render more poorly at the lowest of resolutions as their small details are lost/squished when fitting to the pixel grid.

    Fonts such as Georgia and Droid Serif are notable in that they are specifically designed to be quite well readable even at fairly low resolutions on a screen. But if you consider a serif typeface that works excellently in print, like Caslon or Garamond, these are an atrocious mess at low resolutions on-screen. Whereas in print, I would claim Adobe Caslon to be one of the most readable fonts out there. Probably harking back to point 1 – because it’s so familiar to us.

    Higher resolution screens (the “retina display” trend) are also lessening the need to worry about fitting type to the pixel grid, putting traditional serifs once more onto an even footing (on such displays).

Serif type pre-dates sans-serif and the serifs are originally imitations of chiseled type in stone which then went on to be used just for aesthetic reasons. It was not like there was both serif and sans-serif and one was “chosen” over the other – it would be hundreds of years later before the first sans-serif appeared, and even then it was scorned a little bit at first. So, obviously, if someone were to claim that serifs were “invented” to ease readability, implying that before that all text was sans-serif, then they’re putting the cart before the horse in terms of their understanding of history.

Attribution
Source : Link , Question Author : user56reinstatemonica8 , Answer Author : thomasrutter

Leave a Comment